Untangling Giftedness: IQ Tests, Labels, and What It All Means
Exploring the history, scoring, and shifting definitions behind levels of giftedness
Gifted. Highly gifted. Profoundly gifted. These terms are often used, but the definitions behind them have shifted over time. The numbers and thresholds attached to these labels depend on when you were tested, which test was used, and how scores were calculated. To understand where these definitions come from and why they vary, let’s dive into the history of IQ testing and the frameworks that guide modern interpretations.
IQ Testing in the Early 1900s
In the early 1900s, IQ tests like the Stanford-Binet used the mental age / chronological age × 100 formula to calculate scores (Roid & Barram, 2004). This method allowed for results to soar well beyond what we see today. A 10-year-old performing at the level of a 20-year-old, for instance, could receive an IQ score of 200.
At the time, this approach seemed useful for differentiating levels of intelligence. Individuals scoring 180 or higher were often classified as profoundly gifted. However, there were some issues:
Non-Linear Growth: Mental age doesn’t scale evenly throughout life. The jump from a mental age of 10 to 15 is more significant than a jump from 30 to 35 (Silverman, 2012).
Inflated Scores: The calculation often overestimated ability, especially for younger children outperforming their age group (Gross, 2004).
Cultural and Educational Bias: Tests heavily relied on verbal reasoning and knowledge-based tasks, which reflected a test-taker’s education and environment rather than innate ability (Silverman, 2012).
The Shift to Modern IQ Scoring
By the mid-20th century, IQ tests adopted a new approach: deviation IQ scoring. This method compares an individual’s performance to a norm group of similarly aged peers, making scores more standardized and reliable (Wechsler, 2003). Here’s how it works:
Set Ceilings: Modern IQ tests, like the WISC, cap scores at 160. This ceiling reflects the highest performance that can be reliably measured based on the norm group (Silverman, 2012).
Focus on Deviation: Instead of using mental age, deviation IQ scores show how far an individual’s performance deviates from the average for their age group:
100 = Average
115 = Mildly gifted
130 = Moderately gifted
145 = Highly gifted
160+ = Exceptionally gifted (or profoundly gifted in some contexts)
Subtests and Composite Scoring: Modern IQ tests break cognitive abilities into multiple subtests measuring areas like verbal comprehension, working memory, and processing speed. Each subtest has its own ceiling, usually topping out at raw scores of 18 or 19 (Wechsler, 2003).
The Labels and Their Meaning
Miraca Gross’s framework from Exceptionally and Profoundly Gifted Students provides one of the most widely used breakdowns of giftedness:
Mildly Gifted: IQ 115–129
Moderately Gifted: IQ 130–144
Highly Gifted: IQ 145–159
Exceptionally Gifted: IQ 160–179
Profoundly Gifted: IQ 180 and above (Gross, 2004).
These levels originated during a time when IQ test ceilings reached beyond 160. Today, however, most modern tests cap out at 160, which has reshaped how these labels are applied.
The Davidson Institute has modernized these categories in using IQ 145+ as the benchmark for profoundly gifted, which seems to reflect the practical limitations of today’s deviation IQ scoring while recognizing the rarity and needs of individuals at this level (Davidson Institute for Talent Development, n.d.). Although some sources still reserve "profoundly gifted" for scores of 160+, Davidson’s framework has gained traction for its focus on educational and social-emotional needs.
While there is no official chart, the gifted community still commonly uses Miraca Gross’s chart. There had been discussions about formally updating her chart, however that hasn’t happened.
Why Testing Beyond 160 Is Rare
Modern IQ tests intentionally stop at 160 for several reasons:
Statistical Limitations: Extremely high scores are rare, so there isn’t enough data to validate results beyond this range (Silverman, 2012).
Practicality: Developing tests that measure reliably above 160 is costly, and there’s limited demand (Wechsler, 2003).
Ceiling Effects: Subtests max out, meaning even someone scoring perfectly might only reflect the test’s limits, not their true ability (Roid & Barram, 2004).
Extended norms were introduced to estimate what scores might have been if tests continued. For example, the Stanford-Binet IV included norms up to 210 (Roid & Barram, 2004). However, these extended scores are more of a statistical guess than a precise measurement.
The Takeaway
IQ testing has come a long way, shifting from inflated early scores to the more standardized (and capped) systems we use today. The categories—mildly, moderately, highly, and profoundly gifted—help frame discussions, but they aren’t just about numbers (Robinson et al., 2007).
Individuals scoring above 130 often need specialized instruction and access to intellectual peers, while those scoring above 145 frequently require individualized education plans or alternative schooling. Studies suggest that many children scoring 145+ end up homeschooling at some point because traditional schools aren’t equipped to meet their needs (Silverman, 2012).
Ultimately, the numbers are just a starting point. Whether someone scores 145 or 180, the focus should always be on creating opportunities for growth, connection, and thriving.
References
Gross, M. U. M. (2004). Exceptionally Gifted Children (2nd ed.). Routledge.
Davidson Institute for Talent Development. (n.d.). Profoundly Gifted Students. Retrieved from https://www.davidsongifted.org
Roid, G. H., & Barram, R. A. (2004). Essentials of Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales (SB5) Assessment. Wiley.
Wechsler, D. (2003). WISC-IV Technical and Interpretive Manual. Psychological Corporation.
Silverman, L. K. (2012). Giftedness 101. Springer Publishing Company.
Robinson, A., Shore, B. M., & Enersen, D. L. (2007). Best Practices in Gifted Education: An Evidence-Based Guide. Prufrock Press.
I never know how to describe my eldest because he's 'more gifted' than my gifted younger kid.
It can lead to weird, embarrassing situations like the time we met a gifted playmate and his mum mentioned he'd just got an immunology book. And I was like "oh, so has my son" and it turned out her son was reading a kid's science book whereas mine had just finished the popular immunology book that her husband was reading... Oops.
My daughter was tested with the Stanford Binet where she scored according this as moderately gifted, but acts as exceptionally gifted. Her tester also said that if she weren't so defiant and strong willed, she would've scored higher, but she wasn't a people pleaser like many gifted kids she tested. I don't know if that's something to add to it, I just never thought that the behavior was part of the testing, but I guess I could celebrate how I got out of the people pleasing generational trauma 😅